© 2008 -
2014, Gwen Dewar, Ph.D., all rights reserved
Science supports many of our intuitions about the benefits of play.
Playful behavior appears to have positive effects on the brain and
on a child’s ability to learn. In fact, play may function as an important, if
not crucial, mode for learning.
Want specifics? Here are some examples.
Animal experiments: Play improves memory and stimulates the growth
of the cerebral cortex
In 1964, Marion Diamond and her colleagues published an exciting
paper about brain growth in rats. The neuroscientists had conducted a landmark
experiment, raising some rats in boring, solitary confinement and others in
exciting, toy-filled colonies.
When researchers examined the rats’ brains, they discovered that
the “enriched” rats had thicker cerebral cortices than did the “impoverished”
rats (Diamond et al 1964).
Subsequent research confirmed the results—rats raised stimulating
environments had bigger brains.
They were smarter, too--able to find their way through mazes more
quickly (Greenough and Black 1992).
Do these benefits of play extend to humans? Ethical considerations
prevent us from performing similar experiments on kids. But it seems likely
that human brains respond to play and exploration in similar ways.
Play and exploration trigger the secretion of BDNF, a substance
essential for the growth of brain cells
Again, no one has figured out an ethical way to test this on
humans, so the evidence comes from rats: After bouts of rough-and-tumble play,
rats show increased levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in their
brains (Gordon et al 2003). BDNF is essential for the growth and maintenance of
brain cells. BDNF levels are also increased after rats are allowed to explore
(Huber et al 2007).
Kids pay more attention to academic tasks when they are given
frequent, brief opportunities for free play
Several experimental studies show that school kids pay more
attention to academics after they’ve had a recess--an unstructured break in
which kids are free to play without direction from adults (see Pellegrini and
Holmes 2006 for a review).
There is some circumstantial evidence, too: Chinese and Japanese
students, who are among the best achievers in the world, attend schools that
provide short breaks every 50 minutes (Stevenson and Lee 1990).
Note that physical education classes are not effective substitutes
for free playtime (Bjorkland and Pellegrini 2000).
Physical exercise has important cognitive
benefits in its own right. But physical education classes don't deliver
the same benefits as recess. Researchers suspect that's because PE classes are
too structured and rely too much on adult-imposed rules. To reap all the
benefits of play, a play break must be truly playful.
How long should recess be? No one knows for sure, but there is
some evidence for recesses between 10 and 30 minutes. In a small study of
4-5 year olds, researchers found that recesses of 10 or 20 minutes enhanced
classroom attention. Recesses as long as 30 minutes had the opposite effect
(Pelligrini and Holmes 2006).
Language and the benefits of play
Studies reveal a link between play--particularly symbolic, pretend
play--and the development of language skills. For example:
Psychologist Edward Fisher analyzed 46 published studies of the
cognitive benefits of play (Fisher 1999). He found that “sociodramatic
play”—what happens when kids pretend together—“results in improved performances
in both cognitive-linguistic and social affective domains.”
A study of British children, aged 1-6 years, measured kids’
capacity for symbolic play (Lewis et al 2000). Kids were asked to perform such
symbolic tasks as substituting a teddy bear for an absent object. Researchers
found that kids who scored higher on a test of symbolic play had better
language skills—both receptive language (what a child understands) and
expressive language (the words she speaks). These results remained significant
even after controlling for the age of the child.
Recent research also suggests that playing with blocks contributes
to language development. For more information, see this article about construction
toys and the benefits of play.
Evidence that play promotes creative problem solving
Psychologists distinguish two types of problem--convergent and
divergent. A convergent problem has a single correct solution or answer.
A divergent problem yields itself to multiple solutions.
Some research suggests that the way kids play contributes to their
ability to solve divergent problems.
For instance, in one experiment, researchers presented
preschoolers with two types of play materials (Pepler and Ross 1981). Some kids
were given materials for convergent play (i.e., puzzle pieces). Other kids were
given materials for divergent play (blocks). Kids were given time to play and
then were tested on their ability to solve problems.
The results? Kids given divergent play materials performed better
on divergent problems. They also showed more creativity in their attempts to
solve the problems (Pepler and Ross 1981).
Another experimental study hints at a causal connection between pretend
play (discussed at more length below) and divergent problem-solving ability
(Wyver and Spence 1999). Kids given training in pretend play showed an
increased ability to solve divergent problems, and the converse was true as
well: Kids trained to solve divergent problems showed increased rates of
pretend play.
Make-believe, self-regulation, and reasoning about possible worlds
Divergent problem solving isn't the only cognitive skill linked
with make-believe. Pretend play has also been correlated with two crucial skill
sets--the ability to self-regulate (impulses, emotions, attention) and the ability
to reason counterfactually.
In the first case, studies report that kids who engage in
frequent, pretend play have stronger self-regulation skills. Although more
research is needed to determine if the link is causal (Lillard et al 2013), the
data are consistent with this possibility, and the idea has intuitive appeal.
You can't pretend with another person unless both of you agree about
what you are pretending. So players must conform to a set of rules,
and practice conforming to such rules might help kids develop
better self-control over time.
In the second case, many researchers have noted similarities
between pretend play and counterfactual reasoning, the ability to make
inferences about events that have not actually occurred.
Alison Gopnik and her colleagues (Walker and Gopnik 2013;
Buchsbaum et al 2012) argue that counterfactual reasoning helps us plan and
learn by permitting us to think through "what if" scenarios. Pretend
play taps into the same skill set. So perhaps pretend play provides children
with valuable opportunities to improve their reasoning about possible worlds.
In support of this idea, researchers found evidence of a link
between counterfactual reasoning and pretend play in preschoolers, and this
correlation remained statistically significant even after controlling for a
child's ability to suppress her impulses (Buchsbaum et al 2012).
Math skills and the benefits of play
Here's an intriguing story about play and mathematics:
A longitudinal study measured the complexity of children’s block
play at age 4 and then tracked their academic performance through high school
(Wolfgang, Stannard, & Jones, 2001).
Researchers found that the complexity of block play predicted
kids’ mathematics achievements in high school. In particular, those who had
used blocks in more sophisticated ways as preschoolers had better math grades
and took more math courses (including honors’ courses) as teenagers.
Of course, these results might merely tell us that kids who are
smart in preschool continue to be smart in high school.
But it’s not that simple. The association between block play and
math performance remained even after researchers controlled for a child’s IQ.
It therefore seems plausible that block play itself influenced the cognitive
development of these kids.
Playful experiences are learning experiences
Finally, lest anybody doubt that kids learn through play, we
should keep in mind the following points.
1. Most play involves exploration, and exploration is, by
definition, an act of investigation.
It's easy to see how this applies to a budding scientist who is playing with magnets, but it
also applies to far less intellectual pursuits, like the rough-and-tumble play
in puppies. The animals are testing social bonds and learning how to control
their impulses, so that friendly wrestling doesn't turn into anti-social
aggression. Play is learning.
2. Play is self-motivated and fun.
Thus, anything learned during play is knowledge gained without the
perception of hard work. This is in contrast with activities that we perform as
duties. When learning is perceived to be arduous, our ability to stay focused
may feel like a limited resource that is drained over time (Inzlicht et al
2014). And it's hard to achieve a state of flow, the psychological experience
of being totally, and happily, immersed in what you are doing. Play is an
obvious gateway to the state of flow.
3. These arguments aside, there is also empirical evidence that
kids treat play as a tutorial for coping with real life challenges.
All around the world, children engage in pretend play that
simulates the sorts of activities they will need to master as adults (Lancy
2008), suggesting such play is a form of practice. And when kids are fed
information during pretend play--from more knowledgeable peers or adults--they
take it in. Experiments on American preschoolers suggest that children as young
as 3 understand make distinctions between realistic and fanciful pretending,
and use information learned from realistic pretend scenarios to understand the
real world (Sutherland and Friedman 2012; 2013).
The takeaway? Giving children play-breaks and making children's
academic lessons more playful isn't mere sugar-coating. It might be a way to
enhance kids' natural capacities for intense, self-motivated learning.
No comments:
Post a Comment